ISSN : 2146-3123
E-ISSN : 2146-3131

Jason Theola1, Murti Andriastuti2
1Universitas Indonesia Faculty of Medicine, Jakarta, Indonesia
2Department of Child Health, Universitas Indonesia, Cipto Mangunkusumo Hospital, Jakarta, Indonesia
DOI : 10.4274/balkanmedj.galenos.2025.2024-11-24
Pages : 108-120

Abstract

Background: Numerous studies have reported neurodevelopmental disorders in children with a history of early-life iron deficiency (ID), though findings vary.

Aims: To evaluate the long-term impact of early childhood ID on neurodevelopmental outcomes.

Study Design: Systematic review.

Methods: A literature search was conducted across five electronic databases (PubMed, Cochrane, Scopus, Sage, and Embase) using the keywords “iron deficiency anemia” and “infant.” The JBI critical appraisal tool for cohort studies was used to evaluate study quality.

Results: Seventeen relevant cohort studies were identified through the systematic search. Of these, 14 were rated as high quality, while 3 were classified as moderate quality. The neurodevelopmental domains assessed included cognitive deficits (seven studies), motor deficits (four studies), verbal deficits (seven studies), behavioral deficits (nine studies), auditory function (one study), and neuroendocrine function (two studies).

Conclusion: Early-life ID disrupts neurodevelopment, leading to persistent cognitive, motor, behavioral, and neuroendocrine impairments. Children with a history of early childhood ID demonstrate poorer cognitive, motor, and behavioral outcomes compared with their non-ID counterparts. Preventing ID within the first 1,000 days of life is essential to mitigate irreversible deficits in motor, cognitive, and behavioral functions.


INTRODUCTION

Sufficient nutrition intake during the first 1,000 days of life, from conception to the second year, is essential for optimal neurodevelopment.1 During this period, children have the highest iron demands.2 Early childhood is marked by rapid growth and development, making iron requirements between 6 and 24 months greater than at any other stage of life.3 Inadequate iron intake during this critical phase increases the risk of iron deficiency (ID) in infants and young children. ID accounts for approximately 30-50% of anemia cases in children.4 Iron plays a vital role in brain development by supporting myelination, neurotransmitter synthesis, and oxygen transport in hemoglobin.5 ID within the first 1,000 days of life can lead to long-term, irreversible impairments in motor function, cognition, and behavior.2 A systematic review evaluating iron status in pregnant women found that low maternal iron levels, particularly during the third trimester, are linked to neurodevelopmental disorders in offspring, including deficits in behavior, cognition, and academic performance.6 Therefore, ID during pregnancy can adversely affect fetal brain development.7

ID results from a reduction in iron stores and can progress to iron deficiency anemia (IDA) if left uncorrected. It is identified by low ferritin levels while hemoglobin and normal mean corpuscular volume (MCV) remain within normal ranges. The subsequent stage, ID without anemia, is marked by low ferritin and MCV levels but normal hemoglobin levels. The final stage, IDA, is characterized by low levels of hemoglobin, MCV, and ferritin.8 Although anemia is a severe outcome of ID, ID without anemia can also affect a child’s brain development even before anemia occurs.9,10

In humans, the brain, particularly the hippocampus, undergoes rapid development from late pregnancy to approximately 2-3 years of age. During this period, iron utilization increases alongside the formation of new neurons, dendrites, myelin, synapses, and neurotransmitters. Hippocampus-dependent memory begins to develop and strengthen between 3 and 18 months. The generation of new neurons in the hippocampus occurs at a much faster rate before birth and in the early postnatal years. These neurons integrate into neural networks and are thought to play a crucial role in learning and memory. Consequently, during these critical developmental stages, environmental factors such as ID, which can impair neuronal growth or maturation, may influence both immediate and long-term behavior.11

Early childhood ID is thought to impair attention, memory, and motor skills, even when iron supplementation is provided. These deficits primarily affect the prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, and sensorimotor regions.12-16 Research has also shown that early childhood ID disrupts not only cognitive and motor functions but also verbal, behavioral, emotional, and psychosocial development.17-28 Additionally, several studies have reported differences in neuroendocrine activity between children with a history of early childhood ID and those without.19,24

Several studies have documented neurodevelopmental disorders in children with a history of early childhood ID, though their findings have varied considerably. This systematic review aims to evaluate the long-term impact of early childhood ID on neurodevelopmental outcomes in children.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Ethics statement

This systematic review was registered in an international database (International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews, CRD42024559247). As this study is a systematic review, informed consent and ethics committee approval were not required.

Eligibility criteria

The clinical question was structured using the PICO framework: Population, children under 18 years of age; issue (I), diagnosed with ID within the first 2 years of life; comparators, children with no history of childhood ID; and outcomes, neurodevelopmental impairments identified after a minimum follow-up of 1 year. Studies were included if they met the following criteria: (a) pediatric patients (under 18 years) diagnosed with ID within the first 2 years of life, (b) cohort study design, (c) long-term follow-up of more than 1 year, (d) reporting neurodevelopmental outcomes, (e) published in English, and (f) full text available. Exclusion criteria were as follows: (a) presence of comorbidities other than ID, (b) insufficient data, (c) not published in English, or (d) full text unavailable.

Search strategy

This systematic review was conducted following the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses (PRISMA) guidelines.29 The literature search was carried out from June 13th to 20th, 2023, using keywords aligned with the PICO framework. A Boolean search using OR and AND operators was conducted across five databases: PubMed, Cochrane, Scopus, Sage, and Embase, with the keywords “ID anemia” and “infant.” The Medical Subject Headings (MeSH) terms and search strategy are shown in Table 1. Filters were applied to limit the results to clinical trials, randomized clinical trials (RCTs), and research articles only. An example of the PubMed search query is as follows: ((“ID anemia”[All Fields] OR “anemia, iron deficiency”[MeSH Terms] OR (“anemia”[All Fields] AND “iron deficiency”[All Fields]) OR “iron-deficiency anemia”[All Fields] OR (“iron”[All Fields] AND “deficiency”[All Fields] AND “anemia”[All Fields]) OR “ID anemia”[All Fields]) AND (“infant”[MeSH Terms] OR “infant”[All Fields] OR “infants”[All Fields] OR “infant s”[All Fields])) AND (clinicaltrial[Filter] OR randomizedcontrolledtrial[Filter]).

Study selection process

The initial database search and screening were conducted independently by two authors (J.T. and M.A.). Title and abstract screening were also done independently by both authors to identify studies with eligible designs that addressed the clinical question (specifically cohort studies). Duplicate articles were removed through multiple rounds of screening. Full-text selection was based on the availability of complete articles. Studies were included if all authors agreed they met the eligibility criteria. Disagreements were resolved through consensus, and if consensus could not be reached, the first author made the final decision.

Data extraction and outcome domains

Two authors independently extracted data from the included studies. For each study, we recorded the first author’s name, study design, study location, population at the start (both exposed and control groups), age at ID diagnosis, iron therapy (if applicable), follow-up duration, outcomes, measurement tools, and statistical significance of the results. The data were entered into Microsoft Excel. To minimize the extraction bias, data extraction was carried out independently by both authors. If disagreements could not be resolved through discussion, the first author made the final decision. We extracted data on all neurodevelopmental domains affected by ID: cognitive, motor (both gross and fine), verbal, auditory, behavior, and neuroendocrine.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis was not performed, as this study is a systematic review with heterogeneous data outcomes, making meta-analysis unsuitable. Funnel plot analysis was also not applicable due to the inability to conduct meta-analysis. To minimize publication bias, we followed the PRISMA guidelines for systematic analysis across multiple databases. The cohorts and longitudinal RCT studies were assessed using the JBI critical appraisal tool for cohort studies.30 A study was classified as high quality (low risk of bias) if the score was > 80%, moderate quality (moderate risk of bias) if the score was between 50% and 80%, and low quality (high risk of bias) if the score was < 50%. In case of differing opinions among the authors, further discussion was held to reach a consensus. If disagreements persisted, the first author made the final decision.

RESULTS

The search and selection process is outlined in Figure 1. Out of 3,142 retrieved articles, 17 relevant studies were included in this systematic review. Based on the quality assessment using the JBI critical appraisal tool, 3 studies were of moderate quality with a moderate risk of bias,24,25,31 while the remaining 14 studies were of high quality with a low risk of bias15,17-23,26-28,32-34 (Table 2). The studies included in this review showed high heterogeneity, as they were conducted in various locations and examined different neurodevelopment domains using diverse measurement tools. As a result, a qualitative analysis was performed by summarizing the extracted data in a summary table (Table 3).

ID was diagnosed as early as birth and as late as age 24. Several studies reported iron supplementation durations ranging from at least 3 months27 to 6 months or up to 1 year, depending on the child’s age.19,20,26,32,33 Follow-up and assessment were conducted as early as age 4 years25,32,33 and as late as age 17 years.28 All studies met the inclusion and exclusion criteria. The tools used to assess neurodevelopmental outcomes varied across the studies. The outcomes assessed included not only cognitive functions but also behavior, verbal skills, motor skills, and neuroendocrine function. This systematic review found that ID in early life leads to long-term neurodevelopmental impairments.

Cognitive function in children with a history of early childhood ID was assessed at ages 4 years,32 8-9 years,20 and 10 years.24,26 Seven studies evaluated cognitive function as one of the neurodevelopmental aspects.15,20,22-24,26,32 Of these, three studies found significantly lower cognitive function in children with early childhood ID,24,26,32 one study did not report significance,20 and three studies found no significant differences.15,22,23

Several studies also reported motor function impairments in children with early childhood ID, assessed at ages 4 years,33 5 years,15 6 years,31 and 5-12 years.34 Four studies evaluated motor function using different methods and found that children with a history of early childhood ID had lower motor function compared to those without ID.15,31,33,34 Among these studies, one assessed fine motor performance only,31 one assessed both gross and fine motor functions,15 one assessed overall motor scores without specifying gross or fine motor,34 and one assessed motor activity during daytime sleep.33

In this systematic review, verbal function in children with a history of early childhood ID was assessed at ages 5 years,27 8-9 years,20 and 10 years.24,26 Seven out of the seventeen studies included in this review evaluated verbal function.15,20,22-24,26,27 Among these, two studies found that verbal function was significantly lower in children with early childhood ID compared to those without ID,24,27 two studies showed similar results without stating significance,20,26 and the remaining studies found no significant differences.15,22,23 Regarding auditory function, one study found that children with early childhood ID had normal hearing function at age 10.24

Behavioral problems were assessed in the included studies at ages 4 years,25 5 years,17,18,22 6 years,23 8-9 years,21 10 years,26 and 11-17 years.28 The behavioral parameters varied across studies. Of the nine studies evaluating behavioral problems, seven reported significantly higher behavioral issues in children with a history of early childhood ID.17,18,22,23,25,26,28 One study reported lower behavioral performance in children with ID but did not state significance,21 while only one study found no significant differences in behavior between children with and without early childhood ID.24

Regarding neuroendocrine function, one study found that 10-year-old children with early childhood ID had significantly lower serum cortisol levels compared to normal children.19 In contrast, another study reported higher morning salivary cortisol levels in children with a history of early childhood ID at the same age, compared to those without ID.24

DISCUSSION

Cognitive outcomes encompass various aspects, including memory, overall intelligence quotient, non-verbal knowledge, comprehension, learning, and mathematical abilities. The studies included in this review found that children with a history of early childhood ID exhibited lower cognitive function when assessed at ages 4 years,32 8-9 years,20 and 10 years.24,26 These children showed a slower rate of cognitive improvement compared to those without a history of ID.26 This delay may be linked to myelin dysfunction caused by ID, as myelin formation is a prolonged process that can take months or years in different brain regions.35,36 Disturbances in early brain development can have long-lasting effects, and disruptions in iron homeostasis during neurodevelopment may alter myelin composition, leading to irreversible changes despite treatment. Even children with a history of early childhood ID who did not experience anemia still demonstrated significantly lower cognitive function than those without a history of ID.24 However, several studies noted lower cognitive function in children with a history of ID, though the differences were not statistically significant.15,20,22,23 Verbal function was also significantly impacted by childhood ID24 with most studies indicating lower verbal abilities in children with a history of ID, even if the differences were not statistically significant.20,22,23,26,27

The auditory brainstem and visual evoked potential responses were slower in children with a history of early childhood ID, even after receiving therapy.32 As mentioned earlier, disruptions in myelination lead to developmental disorders that manifest later. Oligodendrocytes, which are crucial for myelination, rely on iron availability for proper functioning. Impaired iron availability during development affects myelination.37 The latency of auditory brainstem response and visual evoked potential is influenced by the conduction velocity during axonal myelination.32 Regarding auditory function, one study found that children with a history of early childhood ID had normal hearing,24 suggesting that the delayed brainstem auditory response was likely due to central nervous system dysfunction caused by early childhood ID.32

Several studies included in this review reported motor impairments in children with a history of childhood ID when assessed at ages 4 years,33 5 years,15 6 years,31 and 5-12 years.34 The previously discussed myelination disorders also impact the corticospinal tract, which affects motor skills.38 The corticospinal tract forms pyramidal tract fibers that connect the sensory and motor areas of the cerebral cortex to the spinal cord, serving as the main pathway for motor signals from the brain to the limbs.39 Myelination of the pyramidal tract begins in early life and is completed within a few years.40 One study specifically examined the relationship between early childhood ID and motor activity during daytime sleep, finding that children with early childhood ID exhibited more leg movements and higher intra-individual variability (IIV) compared to normal children, indicating a neurodevelopmental delay.33 Elevated IIV suggests a delay, as IIV typically decreases with age.41 Full myelination of the central nervous system reduces IIV in the sensorimotor system, while the elimination of transient central nervous system connections also decreases IIV.42 Since iron is essential for myelination, the IIV observed in children with early childhood ID is linked to impaired central nervous system maturation due to myelination disorders.33

ID in infancy is believed to impact social-emotional development during adolescence through both psychosocial and neurobiological pathways.28 The effect of early childhood ID on brain development depends on the timing of the deficiency and the specific brain regions requiring iron at that time.43 Neurotransmitters such as dopamine, serotonin, and norepinephrine regulate neuronal signaling, influencing behavior as well as cognitive and emotional processes in infants.22 Damage to dopamine terminals during the neonatal phase leads to lifelong hyperreactivity to new objects and environments44 and is thought to have long-term effects on attention and emotional responses.45 However, another hypothesis worth considering, which may warrant further research, suggests that children with a history of early childhood ID engage less with caregivers, resulting in fewer developmentally supportive interactions. Over time, this leads to reduced social and environmental stimulation, which may present challenges later in life.17 This is significant as behavior and psychosocial development are broad concepts that go beyond neurobiological factors and also encompass psychosocial elements.

ID in early childhood is believed to affect the neuroendocrine hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis later in life. One study found that 10-year-old children with early childhood ID had significantly lower serum cortisol levels compared to normal children. This study also reported lower plasma cortisol concentrations and a blunted stress response in children with a history of early childhood ID.19 However, another study found the opposite result, with children who had early childhood ID showing higher morning salivary cortisol levels compared to their peers without ID.24 The discrepancy between these findings may be due to differences in sample type, assessment timing, and sampling methods. Nevertheless, both studies suggest a potential link between early ID and neuroendocrine function, warranting further investigation.

Our study addresses whether early-life ID has long-term effects despite treatment. However, it has several limitations. This systematic review includes studies using various tools and methods to assess the same outcomes. The populations, study designs, methods, and follow-up durations also varied, which limits the generalizability of the findings and prevents the application of a meta-analysis. Therefore, we suggest that future studies use standardized outcome measures to enable comparisons and meta-analyses. While cognitive and motor impairments are well-documented in several studies, verbal, behavioral, auditory, and neuroendocrine outcomes are less consistently evaluated, and the imbalance in reported findings may introduce reporting bias. Additionally, the assessment times across studies range from as early as age 4 to as late as adolescence (11-17 years), which may introduce bias into our study.

ID in early childhood disrupts neurodevelopmental processes, leading to long-term effects on cognition, motor skills, behavior, and neuroendocrine function. Children with a history of childhood ID were reported to have lower cognitive, motor, behavior, and neuroendocrine functions compared to those without a history of ID. Our study emphasizes the importance of the neurodevelopmental process, as disturbances during this phase, particularly ID, can result in long-term neurological impairments. Therefore, we stress the importance of preventing ID, especially during the first 1,000 days of life, as it can lead to irreversible deficits in motor function, cognitive ability, and behavior.

Ethics Committee Approval: Not applicable.

Informed Consent: Not applicable

Data Sharing Statement: The datasets analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Authorship Contributions: Concept- M.A.; Design- M.A.; Supervision- M.A.; Materials- M.A.; Data Collection or Processing- J.T., M.A.; Analysis or Interpretation- J.T., M.A.; Literature Search- J.T., M.A.; Writing- J.T., M.A.; Critical Review- J.T., M.A.

Conflict of Interest: The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Funding: The authors declared that this study received no financial support.

REFERENCES

  1. Georgieff MK, Ramel SE, Cusick SE. Nutritional influences on brain development. Acta Paediatr. 2018;107:1310-1321.
  2. McCarthy EK, Murray DM, Kiely ME. Iron deficiency during the first 1000 days of life: are we doing enough to protect the developing brain? Proc Nutr Soc. 2022;81:108-118.
  3. Domellöf M, Braegger C, Campoy C, et al.; ESPGHAN Committee on Nutrition. Iron requirements of infants and toddlers. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr. 2014;58:119-129.
  4. World Health Organization. Conclusions and recommendations of the WHO Consultation on prevention and control of iron deficiency in infants and young children in malaria-endemic areas. Food Nutr Bull. 2007;28(4 Suppl):S621-S627.
  5. Chmielewska A, Chmielewski G, Domellöf M, Lewandowski Z, Szajewska H. Effect of iron supplementation on psychomotor development of non-anaemic, exclusively or predominantly breastfed infants: a randomised, controlled trial. BMJ Open. 2015;5:e009441.
  6. Janbek J, Sarki M, Specht IO, Heitmann BL. A systematic literature review of the relation between iron status/anemia in pregnancy and offspring neurodevelopment. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2019;73:1561-1578.
  7. Georgieff MK. Iron deficiency in pregnancy. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2020;223:516-524.
  8. Kessy JP, Philemon RN, Lukambagire A, et al. Iron depletion, iron deficiency, and iron deficiency anaemia among children under 5 years old in Kilimanjaro, Northern Tanzania: a hospital-based cross-sectional study. East Afr Health Res J. 2019;3:42-47.
  9. Domellöf M. Iron requirements in infancy. Ann Nutr Metab. 2011;59:59-63.
  10. Georgieff MK. Iron assessment to protect the developing brain. Am J Clin Nutr. 2017;106(Suppl 6):1588S-1593S.
  11. Radlowski EC, Johnson RW. Perinatal iron deficiency and neurocognitive development. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7:585.
  12. McCann S, Perapoch Amadó M, Moore SE. The role of iron in brain development: a systematic review. Nutrients. 2020;12;2001.
  13. Lukowski AF, Koss M, Burden MJ, et al. Iron deficiency in infancy and neurocognitive functioning at 19 years: evidence of long-term deficits in executive function and recognition memory. Nutr Neurosci. 2010;13:54-70.
  14. Algarín C, Nelson CA, Peirano P, Westerlund A, Reyes S, Lozoff B. Iron-deficiency anemia in infancy and poorer cognitive inhibitory control at age 10 years. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2013;55:453-458.
  15. Lozoff B, Jimenez E, Wolf AW. Long-term developmental outcome of infants with iron deficiency. N Engl J Med. 1991;325:687-694.
  16. Lozoff B, Jimenez E, Hagen J, Mollen E, Wolf AW. Poorer behavioral and developmental outcome more than 10 years after treatment for iron deficiency in infancy. Pediatrics. 2000;105:E51.
  17. East P, Lozoff B, Blanco E, et al. Infant iron deficiency, child affect, and maternal unresponsiveness: Testing the long-term effects of functional isolation. Dev Psychol. 2017;53:2233-2244.
  18. East P, Delker E, Blanco E, Encina P, Lozoff B, Gahagan S. Effect of infant iron deficiency on children’s verbal abilities: the roles of child affect and parent unresponsiveness. Matern Child Health J. 2019;23:1240-1250.
  19. Felt BT, Peirano P, Algarín C, et al. Long-term neuroendocrine effects of iron-deficiency anemia in infancy. Pediatr Res. 2012;71:707-712.
  20. Hossain SJ, Tofail F, Mehrin SF, Hamadani JD. Six-year follow-up of childhood stimulation on development of children with and without anemia. Pediatrics. 2023;151(Suppl 2):e2023060221E.
  21. Hua M, Shi D, Xu W, et al. Differentiation between fetal and postnatal iron deficiency in altering brain substrates of cognitive control in pre-adolescence. BMC Med. 2023;21:167.
  22. McCarthy EK, Murray DM, Hourihane JOB, Kenny LC, Irvine AD, Kiely ME. Behavioral consequences at 5 y of neonatal iron deficiency in a low-risk maternal-infant cohort. Am J Clin Nutr. 2021;113:1032-1041.
  23. Thorisdottir AV, Gunnarsdottir I, Palsson GI, Gretarsson SJ, Thorsdottir I. Iron status and developmental scores in 6-year-olds highlights ongoing need to tackle iron deficiency in infants. Acta Paediatr. 2013;102:914-919.
  24. Yehuda S, Yehuda M. Long lasting effects of infancy iron deficiency--preliminary results. J Neural Transm Suppl. 2006:197-200.
  25. Chang S, Wang L, Wang Y, et al. Iron-deficiency anemia in infancy and social emotional development in preschool-aged Chinese children. Pediatrics. 2011;127:e927-e933.
  26. Congdon EL, Westerlund A, Algarin CR, et al. Iron deficiency in infancy is associated with altered neural correlates of recognition memory at 10 years. J Pediatr. 2012;160:1027-1033.
  27. Corapci F, Radan AE, Lozoff B. Iron deficiency in infancy and mother-child interaction at 5 years. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2006;27:371-378.
  28. Doom JR, Richards B, Caballero G, Delva J, Gahagan S, Lozoff B. Infant iron deficiency and iron supplementation predict adolescent internalizing, externalizing, and social problems. J Pediatr. 2018;195:199-205.e2.
  29. Page MJ, McKenzie JE, Bossuyt PM, et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ. 2021;372:n71.
  30. Barker TH, Stone JC, Sears K, et al. The revised JBI critical appraisal tool for the assessment of risk of bias for randomized controlled trials. JBI Evid Synth. 2023;21:494-506.
  31. Gunnarsson BS, Thorsdottir I, Palsson G, Gretarsson SJ. Iron status at 1 and 6 years versus developmental scores at 6 years in a well-nourished affluent population. Acta Paediatr. 2007;96:391-395.
  32. Algarín C, Peirano P, Garrido M, Pizarro F, Lozoff B. Iron deficiency anemia in infancy: long-lasting effects on auditory and visual system functioning. Pediatr Res. 2003;53:217-223.
  33. Angulo-Barroso RM, Peirano P, Algarin C, Kaciroti N, Lozoff B. Motor activity and intra-individual variability according to sleep-wake states in preschool-aged children with iron-deficiency anemia in infancy. Early Hum Dev. 2013;89:1025-1031.
  34. Shafir T, Angulo-Barroso R, Calatroni A, Jimenez E, Lozoff B. Effects of iron deficiency in infancy on patterns of motor development over time. Hum Mov Sci. 2006;25:821-838.
  35. Paus T, Collins DL, Evans AC, Leonard G, Pike B, Zijdenbos A. Maturation of white matter in the human brain: a review of magnetic resonance studies. Brain Res Bull. 2001;54:255-266.
  36. Ortiz E, Pasquini JM, Thompson K, et al. Effect of manipulation of iron storage, transport, or availability on myelin composition and brain iron content in three different animal models. J Neurosci Res. 2004;77:681-689.
  37. Cheli VT, Correale J, Paez PM, Pasquini JM. Iron Metabolism in oligodendrocytes and astrocytes, implications for myelination and remyelination. ASN Neuro. 2020;12:1759091420962681.
  38. Kato D, Wake H, Lee PR, et al. Motor learning requires myelination to reduce asynchrony and spontaneity in neural activity. Glia. 2020;68:193-210.
  39. Natali AL, Reddy V, Bordoni B. Neuroanatomy, Corticospinal Cord Tract. [Updated, 2023 Aug 14]. In: StatPearls [Internet]. Treasure Island (FL): StatPearls Publishing; 2025 Jan-.
  40. Rothwell J. Ascending and descending pathways of the spinal cord. In: Control of Human Voluntary Movement. Springer, Dordrecht; 1994.
  41. Deutsch KM, Newell KM. Noise, variability, and the development of children’s perceptual-motor skills. Developmental Review. 2005;25:155-180.
  42. Kail R. The neural noise hypothesis: evidence from processing speed in adults with multiple sclerosis. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition. 1997;4:157-165.
  43. Bastian TW, Rao R, Tran PV, Georgieff MK. The effects of early-life iron deficiency on brain energy metabolism. Neurosci Insights. 2020;15:2633105520935104.
  44. Beard JL, Felt B, Schallert T, Burhans M, Connor JR, Georgieff MK. Moderate iron deficiency in infancy: biology and behavior in young rats. Behav Brain Res. 2006;170:224-232.
  45. Roncagliolo M, Garrido M, Walter T, Peirano P, Lozoff B. Evidence of altered central nervous system development in infants with iron deficiency anemia at 6 mo: delayed maturation of auditory brainstem responses. Am J Clin Nutr. 1998;68:683-690.

Viewed : 750
Downloaded : 626